Professor Vincent Connelly
BA (Hons), PhD
Professor & Programme Lead for Psychology
School of Psychology, Social Work and Public Health
Role
Vince is a Professor of Psychology and Programme Lead for Psychology.
As Programme Lead, I am responsible for the strategic development and leadership of the psychology portfolio at Oxford Brookes. I lead an integrated team of academic, technical, research and administrative staff who deliver psychology programmes to circa 400+ students ensuring we hit key legislative, quality assurance, recruitment, teaching, research and financial targets. I also teach on the psychology programmes, supervise doctoral students and have an active programme of research.
Externally, I represent the psychology team at the Association for Heads of Psychology Departments and I am the British Psychological Society Director of Teaching and Learning and Programme Liaison Representative for Oxford Brookes.
Research
I have two main areas of research interest:
Armed Forces organisational psychology research
I have recently begun a programme of research investigating part time military Reservists, Armed Forces Veterans and the organisational psychology of the Armed Forces. This has included funded projects examining Reservists family support, the adoption of a more integrated full time and part time "Whole Force" approach to personnel in the military and the recruitment and retention of Reservists.
I have also carried out some research on veterans and supervised a doctoral student examining the perception of veterans in wider UK society. I am working with colleagues from other UK universities and abroad on this work.
The successful impact of this research on the UK Armed Forces is highlighted in this case study.
Writing and reading research
I also have research interests around the development of writing, spelling and reading. In have instigated and published work on why some children struggle to write, especially those who have been identified with dyslexia. I have held a number of research grants in this area with colleagues from other universities both in the UK and abroad.
Externally Funded Research Grants
- Parry, E., Robinson, D, Morrison, Z & Connelly, V. (2022-2023). Reserve Forces 30 Research Support. HSSRC for MoD.
- Connelly, V., Carding, P., Schilling, S., Morrison, Z. & Bricknell, M. (2020-2022) Rapidly formed COVID-19 teams in the NHS: implications for leadership, team-working, career intentions and individual mental health. Economic and Social Science Research Council.
- Cunningham-Burley, S., Morrison, Z. & Connelly, V. & Loretto, W. (2014-2018). The changing negotiation between civil and military lives as a reservist under Army 2020. Economic and Social Science Research Council/MoD Future Reserves Research Programme (FRRP).
- Connelly, V, Morrison, Z., Fear, N., Hennelly, S. & Smith, J. (2016- 2017). Support to the Families of Reservists: An Investigation of Needs, Current Provision and Gaps. DHCSTC for MoD.
- Parry, E, Connelly, V. Morrison, Z & Robinson, D. (2016). Integration of the Whole Force: Understanding barriers and enablers to maximise task and team performance. DHCSTC for MoD.
- Parry, E, Morrison, Z, Robinson, D. & Connelly, V. (2014-2016). Awareness, Attraction and Retention of the Reserve Forces. DHCSTC for MoD.
- Connelly, V. & Dockrell, J.E. (2011-2012). Developing evidence-based practice through use of a developmental model of writing. Economic and Social Science Research Council.
- Connelly, V, & Dockrell, J.E. (2009-2011) Profiling writing in children with specific language difficulties. Leverhulme Trust.
Recent Research Student Project Topics
- Rita Phillips “Understanding and explaining the public perceptions of UK Veterans”
- Annabel Molyneaux “Teaching, learning and practicing writing at primary school: the effects of writing environments on links between transcription and text generation”
- Lynsey O’Rourke “Spellcheck assistance with spelling accuracy in students with dyslexia”
- Julia Birchenough “The influence of individual differences on reading in readers of consistent compared to inconsistent orthographies”
- Mona Sakr. “The Influence of Computers on Child Conceptions and Adult Practices of the Drawing-Writing Distinction.”
- Emma Sumner. “Children with Dyslexia: Spelling as a constraint on writing.”
Centres and institutes
Groups
Projects
Projects as Co-investigator
- Supporting ALL to thrive: Developing a holistic understanding of the impact of Service life on the education of children with Special Educational Needs and Disabilities (SEND/ASN/ALN) and their families.(01/03/2024 - 31/08/2025), funded by: Armed Forces Covenant Fund Trust, funding amount received by Brookes: £60,000, funded by: Armed Forces Covenant Fund Trust
Publications
Journal articles
-
Connelly V, Hennelly S, Fear NT, Morrison Z, Gribble R, Smith J, 'Reservist families and their understanding of military welfare support as a (non)military family'
Journal of Military, Veteran and Family Health 10 (2) (2024) pp.47-62
ISSN: 2368-7924 eISSN: 2368-7924AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARIntroduction: Many nations rely on volunteer reservists willing to train in their spare time and deploy on military operations. This willingness is influenced by familial support. The authors sought to better understand the expectations of, and experiences with, welfare support to UK reservist families for routine training and deployment. Methods: A bespoke survey for family members of reservists was constructed to investigate awareness, use, and experience of both routine and deployment-related welfare support; 140 family members participated. In addition, 33 semi-structured interviews were conducted and deductively coded. Most participants in the survey and interviews were spouses and parents of part-time reservists. Results: The survey and interviews reported low awareness and use of available family welfare services. Most participants did not know how to access support, even during deployment, and had inconsistent local experiences of welfare support. There was a desire for more welfare information and personal contact with unit welfare staff. The key role of the reservist as a barrier or facilitator of information was highlighted. Discussion: Most families of reservists do not identify as military families, have low awareness of family support and welfare, and do not require routine access to support. This contributes to an under-used family welfare and support system that also suffers from localized unit variation. More access to information online, more contact with better trained welfare staff, and increased reservist awareness of welfare and support should reduce inconsistencies and improve family satisfaction and reservist retention.
-
Schilling S, Armaou M, Morrison Z, Carding P, Bricknell M, Connelly V, '"Trust people you've never worked with": A Social Network Visualization of Teamwork, Cohesion, Social Support and Mental Health in NHS Covid Personnel'
Frontiers in Psychology 15 (2024)
ISSN: 1664-1078 eISSN: 1664-1078AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARBackground: The unprecedented rapid re-deployment of healthcare workers from different care pathways into newly created and fluid COVID-19 teams provides a unique opportunity to examine the interaction of many of the established non-technical factors for successful delivery of clinical care and teamwork in healthcare settings. This research paper therefore aims to address these gaps by qualitatively exploring the impact of COVID work throughout the pandemic on permanent and deployed personnel's experiences, their ability to effectively work together, and the effect of social dynamics (e.g., cohesion, social support) on teamwork and mental health. Methods: Seventy-five interviews were conducted across the UK between March and December 2021 during wave 2 and 3 of COVID-19 with 75 healthcare workers who were either permanent staff on Intensive Care/High Dependency Units used as COVID wards, had been rapidly deployed to such a ward, or had managed such wards. Work Life Balance was measured using the WLB Scale. Interview transcripts were qualitatively coded and thematic codes were compared using network graph modelling.Results: Using thematic network analysis, four overarching thematic clusters were found, (1) Teamwork, (2) Organizational Support & Management, (3) Cohesion & Social Support, and (4) Psychological Strain. The study has three main findings. First, the importance of social factors for teamwork and mental health, whereby team identity may influence perceptions of preparedness, collaboration and communication, and impact on the collective appraisal of stressful events and work stressors. Secondly, it demonstrates the positive and negative impact of professional roles and skills on the development of teamwork and team identity. The study identifies the more pronounced negative impact of COVID work on deployed personnel's workload, mental health, and career intentions, exacerbated by reduced levels of social support during, and after, their deployment. Conclusions: The analysis was able to highlight the interactive and interdependent role of social dynamics on teamwork, suggesting that besides technical skills and preparedness, group membership and perception of professional category may play a bigger role for teamwork than previously stated. It also highlights the potentially protective impact of team-membership on participants experience of their pandemic work, especially considering the length of the recent pandemic.
-
Ben-Shalom U, Reizer A, Connelly V, Rickover I, 'The adaptation of soldiers to post-service life – the mediating impact of political views on the relationship between violence and adaptation'
Frontiers in Psychology 14 (2023)
ISSN: 1664-1078 eISSN: 1664-1078AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARIntroduction: The current research explores the association between political views, combat experiences, and the adaptation of soldiers to post-service life. Violent experiences in military service were explored as contributors to both positive and negative dimensions of adaptation, while political views served as possible mediators.
Methods: Three hundred and twenty Israeli veterans participated in the study.
Results: Political views were correlated with adaptation, especially left-to-right voting and anti-militarism. The results support the mediating role of political beliefs (left–right voting and militarism) in the relationship between combat experience and adaptation to post-service life.
Discussion: We contend that political perceptions affect adaptation through sense-making of the combat experiences and the individual processing of these experiences, and the willingness to continue in reserve service, which allows social support and recognition. In addition, they are linked to a sense of bitterness following the reduction of public participation in military and reserve service.
-
Critten S, Connelly V, Dockrell JE, Mundy I, O'Rourke L, Callaghan L, Walter K, 'Hand-writing processes when spelling morphologically complex words in children with and without Developmental Language Disorder'
Frontiers in Psychology 14 (2023)
ISSN: 1664-1078 eISSN: 1664-1078AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARIntroduction: Representations activated during hand-writing production code information on morphological structure and reflect decomposition of the root and suffix. Children with Developmental Language Disorder (DLD) have significant difficulties in spelling morphologically complex words, but previous research has not sought evidence for a morphological decomposition effect via an examination of hand-writing processes in this population. Method: Thirty-three children aged 9-10 years with DLD, 33 children matched for chronological age (CA), and 33 younger children aged 7-8 years matched for oral language ability (LA) completed a dictated spelling task (21 words; 12 with inflectional suffixes, nine with derivational suffixes). The task was completed on paper with an inking pen linked to a graphics tablet running the hand-writing software Eye and Pen. Pause analyses and letter duration analyses were conducted. Results: The three groups showed similar hand-writing processes, evidencing a morphological decomposition effect in a natural writing task. Pause durations observed at the root/suffix boundary were significantly longer than those occurring in the root. Letter durations were also significantly longer for the letter immediately prior to the boundary compared to the letter after it. Nevertheless, despite being commensurate to their LA matches for mean pause durations and letter durations, children with DLD were significantly poorer at spelling derivational morphemes. Hand-writing processes did significantly predict spelling accuracy but to a much lesser extent compared to reading ability. Discussion: It is suggested that derivational spelling difficulties in DLD may derive more from problems with underspecified orthographic representations as opposed to hand-writing processing differences.
-
Ben-Shalom U, Rickover I, Reizer A, Connelly V, 'Emergent Veteran Identity: Toward a New Theory of Veteran Identity in Israeli Society'
Armed Forces & Society [online first] (2023)
ISSN: 0095-327X eISSN: 1556-0848AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARIsraeli society has seen a gradual decrease in the proportion of compulsory and reserve soldiers amid growing criticism of the military from those who have previously served. This criticism is connected to a willingness on their part to organize collective action for postservice benefits and influence other postservice-related issues. We argue that a new theoretical concept of an “emergent veteran identity” could explain this new social phenomenon for both the Israeli military and others. In this study, 248 Israeli veterans completed questionnaires designed to investigate emergent veteran identity. The results reveal that emergent veteran identity was explained by the perception of the role of the military in society, by the organizational dimensions of veterans’ transition into society, and, to a lesser extent, by combat experiences. Female veterans had a higher emergent veteran identity and exhibited higher transformation limbo. The article also discusses the utility of this new concept for the study of veterans in general and the results’ implications for threats to and the loss of military identity.
-
Stefan Schilling 1 2, Maria Armaou 1 3, Zoe Morrison 4, Paul Carding 5, Martin Bricknell 2, Vincent Connelly 1, 'Understanding teamwork in rapidly deployed interprofessional teams in intensive and acute care: A systematic review of reviews'
PLoS ONE 17 (8) (2022)
ISSN: 1932-6203 eISSN: 1932-6203AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARThe rapid increase of acute and intensive care capacities in hospitals needed during the response to COVID-19 created an urgent demand for skilled healthcare staff across the globe. To upscale capacity, many hospitals chose to increase their teams in these departments with rapidly re-deployed inter-professional healthcare personnel, many of whom had no prior experience of working in a high-risk environment and were neither prepared nor trained for work on such wards. This systematic review of reviews examines the current evidence base for successful teamwork in rapidly deployed interprofessional teams in intensive and acute care settings, by assessing systematic reviews of empirical studies to inform future deployments and support of rapidly formed clinical teams. This study identified 18 systematic reviews for further analysis. Utilising an integrative narrative synthesis process supported by thematic coding and graphical network analysis, 13 themes were found to dominate the literature on teams and teamwork in inter-professional and inter-disciplinary teams. This approach was chosen to make the selection process more transparent and enable the thematic clusters in the reviewed papers to be presented visually and codifying four factors that structure the literature on inter-professional teams (i.e., team-internal procedures and dynamics, communicative processes, organisational and team extrinsic influences on teams, and lastly patient and staff outcomes). Practically, the findings suggest that managers and team leaders in fluid and ad-hoc inter-professional healthcare teams in an intensive care environment need to pay attention to reducing pre-existing occupational identities and power-dynamics by emphasizing skill mix, establishing combined workspaces and break areas, clarifying roles and responsibilities, facilitating formal information exchange and developing informal opportunities for communication. The results may guide the further analysis of factors that affect the performance of inter-professional teams in emergency and crisis deployment.
-
Phillips R, Connelly V, Burgess M, 'How do Sociodemographic Characteristics Influence UK Civilian Opinions of UK Armed Forces Iraq and Afghanistan Veterans?'
Armed Forces & Society 49 (2) (2022) pp.419-445
ISSN: 0095-327X eISSN: 1556-0848AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADAREvidence suggests that UK veterans are seen as victims with a consequent elevation of concern for their perceived mental health needs. The present study examined sociodemographic factors that contribute to victimising conceptualisations of British Army Iraq and Afghanistan veterans. Participants (N=234) provided three word-associations to “British Army Iraq Veteran” and “British Army Afghanistan Veteran” and answered sociodemographic questions. A Multiple Linear Regression outlines that low national pride, opposition towards the missions and higher levels of education predict elevated levels of victimizing word-associations. Narrative accounts from 21 interviewed UK participants suggest that participants who did not perceive the recent conflicts as legitimate conceptualise veterans as passive, naïve actors who had to submit to the anthropomorphic agency of the government. This allowed holding overtly appreciative though belittling attitudes towards veterans, while opposing the missions. To dissociate veterans from victimizing perceptions, better knowledge about service and justifications for deployments need to be provided.
-
Phillips R, Connelly V, Burgess M, 'Exploring the Victimization of British Veterans: Comparing British Beliefs about Veterans with Beliefs about Soldiers'
Armed Forces & Society 48 (2) (2022) pp.385-409
ISSN: 0095-327X eISSN: 1556-0848AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADAREvidence suggests that most of the UK public appreciate currently serving UK Armed Forces personnel but are less positive in their beliefs about veterans. This research examined the social representations held by civilian participants of UK veterans and serving soldiers to understand why veterans may be seen more negatively. An open-ended word-association task was completed by 234 UK participants where they were asked to provide three initial responses to the words ‘Veteran’ and ‘Soldier’ and to evaluate their responses in accordance to prototypicality. The 1404 resultant associations were grouped into 14 thematic clusters. Using the Hierarchical Evocation Method, the results suggest ‘Heroizing Associations’ to be a defining core-element for ‘Soldier’ and ‘Veteran’ but ‘Victimizing Associations’ to be an element only for ‘Veteran’. Principal Component Analyses suggest ‘Victimizing Associations’ are related to war and deindividuated associations; ‘Heroizing Associations’ are related to characterizations of the veteran’s personality. Implications and future directions are discussed.
-
Walter, K, Dockrell J, Connelly V, 'A sentence-combining intervention for struggling writers: response to intervention'
Reading and Writing 34 (2021) pp.1825-1850
ISSN: 0922-4777 eISSN: 1573-0905AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARChildren who struggle with writing are a heterogeneous group and may experience difficulties in a range of domains, including spelling, reading, and oral language. These difficulties are reflected in their writing and may influence their responsiveness to writing interventions. The effectiveness of a targeted sentence-combining intervention to improve the writing skills of 71 struggling writers, aged 7 to 10 years, was compared with a spelling intervention and a business as usual (waiting list) control condition. Some struggling writers also performed poorly on measures of reading and oral language. Children's performance on a range of writing measures were assessed at baseline (t1), immediate post-test (t2) and delayed post-test (t3). Children receiving the sentence-combining intervention showed significant improvements in the sentence combining measure at t2 and t3 compared to both the spelling intervention and waiting list controls. Exploratory regression analyses found that children in the sentence-combining intervention, with a low t1 sentence combining score, low reading skills or better t1 spelling skills, were more likely to show improvements at t2. Findings indicate that when devising interventions for struggling writers, specific profiles of skills should be considered. Specifically, sentence combining may be more appropriate for SWs whose primary area of difficulty is reading, rather than poor spelling or oral language.
-
Phillips R, Connelly V, Burgess M, 'Representations of British Armed Forces Veterans in the Press: A Quantitative Analysis of Newspaper Articles'
Journal of Political & Military Sociology 47 (1) (2020) pp.77-98
ISSN: 2642-2190AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARPrevious research has shown that British public perception of veterans can be negative and erroneous. Surveys, for example, indicate that veterans are characterized as skilled and valorous individuals but also as suffering from ill-health, unemployment and homelessness. To investigate how these beliefs may form, the present study examines the public dimension of knowledge by analyzing depictions of veterans in the media. A total of 335 newspaper articles that represent UK veterans were downloaded from Google News and UKPressonline. After the articles were classified with a content analysis, chi-squared tests were conducted to understand how the representation of veterans may be related to the political affiliations of news agencies
and by newspaper format. The results suggest that UK veterans are predominantly represented in both heroic and victimizing contexts. While political affiliation did not affect the representation of veterans significantly, newspaper format did, with broadsheet newspapers emphasizing victimized contexts and local and international newspapers focusing on heroic contexts. Tabloids are a symbolic middle ground between broadsheet, international and local formats. In conclusion, broadsheet newspapers may express sociocultural scrutiny towards the ways in which veterans are treated while local and international newspapers may focus on stereotypical representations of heroic British military actions that veterans may symbolize. Implications and conclusions are discussed. -
Connelly V, 'Understanding and explaining the marginalization of part time British Army Reservists'
Armed Forces & Society 47 (4) (2020) pp.661-689
ISSN: 0095-327X eISSN: 1556-0848AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARRecent changes in the British Army mean part time Reservists and full time Regulars need to become better integrated. However, there has been a long history of workplace tensions between the full time and part time elements in the British Army. This mirrors those found in many civilian workplaces. Focus group data with 105 full time Regular British Army soldiers confirmed that time and emotional commitment are strongly linked in a full time professional workplace that has strong, definite and enduring boundaries. This, alongside demands for conformity and stratification by rank explained the high risk of marginalization of part time Reservists. The legitimacy of part time Reservists, especially in the combat arms, was often challenged. Using this explanatory framework some implications and practical ways that tensions may be reduced between full time and part time members of the British Army, and other Armed Forces facing similar tensions, were highlighted.
-
Phillips R, Burgess M, Connelly V, 'Explaining the Relationship between Religiosity and Increased Wellbeing: Avoidance of Identity Threat as a Key Factor'
Journal of Beliefs & Values 42 (2) (2020) pp.163-176
ISSN: 1361-7672AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARPrevious research outlines a relationship between religiosity and increased mental and physical wellbeing. However, to date findings from quantitative and qualitative research do not offer an unambiguous explanation for this relationship. The study addresses this gap in knowledge by examining underlying identity construction processes in relation to religiosity and wellbeing in the light of Identity Process Theory (Breakwell, 1986, 2001, 2010). Eight Christian converts from different deep-faith groups were recruited via purposive and modified snowball sampling. Detailed descriptions of first-hand experiences were collected by biographic-narrative interviews and analysed by Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis. The findings suggest that religious elements are integrated into identity content as these respond to a set of motivational principles of identity construction. In doing so, religious elements in identity content contribute to the avoidance of identity threat and to the maintenance of a positive identity structure. The adoption of health-promoting behaviours that relate to the application of beliefs (i.e. practice forgiveness, love oneself and the other, stopping excessive drinking) are consequences thereof and improve mental and physical wellbeing. In conclusion, the relationship between religiosity and wellbeing may be explained by the way in that religious elements in identity impact on identity maintenance process.
-
O'Rourke L, Connelly V, Barnett, AL, Afonso O, 'Use of spellcheck in text production by college students with dyslexia'
Journal of Writing Research 12 (1) (2020) pp.35-62
ISSN: 2030-1006AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARIt is widely assumed that by identifying spelling errors and suggesting replacement words, spellcheck allows writers to revise spelling errors even if they do not have the necessary spelling knowledge. However, there have been no studies evaluating the efficacy of modern spellcheck tools for students with spelling difficulties, such as dyslexia. In fact, the very limited and dated research into use of spellcheck by writers with dyslexia indicated that, even when using spellcheck to revise spelling errors, this group left many misspellings in their texts. The current study is the first to investigate whether a modern spellcheck program allows college students with dyslexia to produce texts that are as free from misspellings as texts by their peers, and whether this affects the quality of the text in other ways.
College students with dyslexia (n=18) and a control group of peers (n=18) wrote two short essays using Microsoft Word, one with spellcheck active and one without spellcheck active. Spelling accuracy and overall quality of the texts were measured. Without spellcheck, students with dyslexia made more misspellings than the control group, however, with spellcheck active students from both groups left almost zero misspelled words in their texts. Text quality was not affected. Results demonstrate that spellcheck helps college students with dyslexia to overcome the limitations that poor spelling knowledge imposes. Importantly, results indicate that spellcheck does not lead to improvements in text beyond spelling accuracy, or lead to poorer quality texts, indicating that it is suitable for use in exam conditions.
-
Sumner E, Connelly V, 'Writing and Revision Strategies of Students with and without Dyslexia'
Journal of Learning Disabilities 53 (3) (2019) pp.189-198
ISSN: 0022-2194 eISSN: 1538-4780AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARPrevious work suggests that written text produced by university students with dyslexia is scored lower than that produced by their peers. The present study used a digital writing tablet to examine the writing process and the quality of text written by university students with dyslexia. Revision behaviour during and after writing was also investigated. Thirty-two university students with dyslexia (mean age, 20 years), were compared to 32 typically-developing (TD) students matched by age. Students composed a written text in response to an expository essay prompt. In line with previous research, students with dyslexia made a higher number of spelling errors and their essays were rated as poorer than TD students. However, students with dyslexia were comparable to their peers on measures of time spent writing, amount of text produced, and the temporal analyses (handwriting execution, pause times). Students with dyslexia made significantly more revisions to spelling during and after transcription than their peers, although other revision behaviour was similar across groups. Explanations for the finding of poor writing quality are explored. Importantly, the findings suggest that continued support with spelling and writing is needed for university students with dyslexia. Instruction directed towards effective revision strategies may also prove useful. Limitations and directions for future research are discussed.
-
Barnett AL, Connelly V, Miller B, 'The Interaction of Reading, Spelling and Handwriting Difficulties with Writing Development'
Journal of Learning Disabilities 53 (2) (2019) pp.92-95
ISSN: 0022-2194 eISSN: 1538-4780Published here Open Access on RADAR -
Stuart NJ, Connelly V, Dockrell JE, 'Written verb use and diversity in children with Developmental Language Disorder: Stepping stones to academic writing'
Reading and Writing 33 (1) (2019) pp.67-96
ISSN: 0922-4777 eISSN: 1573-0905AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARVerb use and the production of verb argument structure in the written texts of children in elementary school is a key stepping stone towards academic writing success that has remained relatively unexplored and is a notable gap in our understanding of writing development. To evaluate the role of verbs in the written narrative texts of children, we compared verb use in 10 year old children that had specific weaknesses in oral language, those with Developmental Language Disorder (DLD), and samples of children of the same age (CA) and the same raw scores on an oral language task (language ability or LAb). Standardised measures of oral language, reading fluency, and spelling were completed. Participants then completed a standardised writing task and the texts were examined for verb argument structure, verb production and verb diversity. No between-group differences were found in the written narrative texts in relation to the production of verb argument structures. By contrast, the number of verbs produced, and the number of different verbs used differed significantly. The total number of verbs and number of different verbs produced by the children with DLD was commensurate with their LAb peers but not their CA matched peers. All children relied on a small group of high frequency verbs in their writing, although there was evidence of greater verb diversity in the older typically developing children. Verbs produced and their diversity in narrative writing was predicted by both an oral language formulated sentences task and reading fluency, thus demonstrating the close links between expressive oral language, reading, and writing production in all children.
-
Dockrell JE, Connelly V, Arfe B, 'STRUGGLING WRITERS IN ELEMENTARY SCHOOL: CAPTURING DRIVERS OF PERFORMANCE'
Learning and Instruction 60 (2019) pp.75-84
ISSN: 0959-4752AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARConceptualising the difficulties experienced by struggling writers in middle elementary school is of both theoretical and practical importance. To further our understanding of the problems experienced by struggling writers we aimed to identify the writing measure which best discriminated struggling writers from their peers, and the proximal and distal factors which which contributed to performance. The performance of 96 students (Mean age 0;4), 39 of whom were independently identified as struggling writers using a norm referenced standardised test, was examined at word, sentence, and text level. Standardised measures of transcription, oral language and working memory were collected. The extent to which independent product and process writing measures accurately identified the students was tested using ROC analyses. The skills which underpinned performance were examined using regression analyses and path analysis. Written sentence generation was the most sensitive and specific measure to identify struggling writers at this point in development and, was concurrently predicted by both oral sentence level skills, handwriting fluency and listening span. Path models demonstrated that oral language contributed both directly and indirectly to sentence level writing. Implications for developmental models of writing and support for struggling writers are discussed.
-
Limpo T, Alves RA, Connelly V, 'Testing the Effectiveness of Handwriting Interventions: : Introduction to the special issue'
Reading and Writing 31 (6) (2018) pp.1249-1253
ISSN: 0922-4777 eISSN: 1573-0905AbstractThis special issue was born within the COST Action IS1401ELN – Strengthening Europeans’ Capabilities by Establishing the European Literacy Network, with the major goal of disseminating evidence-based practices to promote handwriting and motivate researchers to move this research field forward. This issue includes five articles testing a different range of interventions to promote handwriting in students with and without disabilities from kindergarten to middle grades, across multiple contexts (viz., United States, Switzerland, Belgium, and Portugal). A final commentary paper closes the special issue with a discussion on the importance of acquiring fluent and legible handwriting along with the contributions and limitations of the investigations presented in the special issue.Published here Open Access on RADAR -
Sakr M, Connelly V, Wild M, 'Imitative or Iconoclastic? How young children use ready-made images in digital art'
International Journal of Art and Design Education 37 (1) (2018) pp.41-52
ISSN: 1476-8062 eISSN: 1476-8070AbstractDigital art-making tends to foreground the inclusion of ready-made images in children's art. While some lament children's use of such images, suggesting that they constrain creativity and expression, others have argued that ready-made digital materials offer children the opportunity to create innovative and potentially iconoclastic artefacts through processes of ‘remix’ and ‘mash-up’. In order to further this debate, observations are needed to explore the different ways that children use ready-made images in their digital art and the various purposes that these images can serve. Adopting a social semiotic perspective, this article offers an in-depth examination of five episodes of 4–5 year-olds’ digital art-making that collectively demonstrate the diversity of approaches that young children take towards the inclusion of ready-made images in their digital art-making. The article discusses these findings in relation to suggestions for what adults can do to support children to adopt a playful and critically aware approach to the use of ready-made images in digital art-making.Published here Open Access on RADAR -
Limpo T, Alves RA, Connelly V, 'Examining the Transcription-Writing Link: Effects of Handwriting Fluency and Spelling Accuracy on Writing Performance via Planning and Translating in Middle Grades'
Learning and Individual Differences: Journal of Psychology and Education 53 (2017) pp.26-36
ISSN: 1041-6080 eISSN: 1873-3425AbstractThis study examined the relationships between transcription, high-level writing processes, and writing performance in Grades 7–8 (N = 196). Structural equation modeling was used to test the direct effects of handwriting fluency and spelling accuracy on planning and translating, and of these latter on writing performance, as well as the indirect effects of handwriting fluency and spelling accuracy on writing performance, via planning and translating. Results showed that the proposed model fitted the data extremely well, explaining 46% of the variability in writing performance. We found that higher handwriting fluency was associated with better planning skills, higher spelling accuracy was associated with better translating skills, and better planning and translating skills were associated with greater writing performance. We found indirect effects of handwriting fluency and spelling accuracy on writing performance, respectively, via planning and translating. These results suggest that transcription continues to impact on writing during teenage school years, by constraining high-level writing processes.Published here Open Access on RADAR -
Birchenough JM, Davies R, Connelly V, 'Rated age-of-acquisition norms for over 3,200 German words.'
Behavior Research Methods 49 (2) (2016) pp.484-501
ISSN: 1554-351X eISSN: 1554-3528AbstractWords that have been learned early in life are responded to faster than words that have been acquired later. Subjective ratings of acquisition ages have been successfully employed to study the effect of age of acquisition (AoA). Although a large number of norms exist in many languages, fewer are available for German. Therefore, subjective AoA ratings for 3,259 German words were collected online, including 2,363 nouns and 473 verbs. These words were presented in lists of 140 words, and participants rated the age in years at which they had first learned each word. A split-half correlation testified to a high internal reliability. There were also high correlations with rated AoA values for subsets of the items that had been collected in previous studies, in both German and English. Age and gender were found to influence the ratings very weakly, in that older and male participants tended to give slightly higher age ratings. Education, multilingualism, and frequent usage of languages other than German did not exert an influence on the rating values. These new ratings will extend the currently existing norms available for language and reading research across languages and will provide researchers with a wider choice of word stimuli. The ratings are available expressed in two measurements: age in years, and AoA rated on a 7-point Likert scale.Published here Open Access on RADAR -
Sakr M, Connelly V, Wild M, '"Evil cats" and "jelly floods": young children’s collective constructions of digital art-making in the early years classroom'
Journal of Research in Childhood Education 30 (1) (2016) pp.128-141
ISSN: 0256-8543 eISSN: 2150-2641AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARDigital technologies have the potential to offer new opportunities for children’s expressive arts practices. While adult expectations surround and shape children’s visual art-making on paper in the early years classroom, such expectations are not so established in relation to digital art-making. So how do children make sense of digital art-making when it is newly introduced into the classroom and adult input is minimal? Drawing on a social semiotic ethnographic perspective, this paper explores this question by examining instances of 4-5 year olds’ spoken dialogue around the computer during a week in which digital art-making was first introduced into the classroom. Analysis focused on interactions where children proposed, reinforced or challenged conceptions of digital art-making. These interactions demonstrated that children’s digital art-making was negotiated and constructed through particular processes. Three such processes are presented here: the use of collective motifs and metaphors; attributing ‘expert’ status; and polarizing conflicts. Understanding these processes offers a starting point for thinking about how a new activity like digital art-making can be integrated into the early years classroom and supported by practitioners.
-
Alstad Z, Sanders E, Abbott RD, Barnett A, Henderson S, Connelly V, Berninger V, 'Modes of alphabet letter production during middle childhood and adolescence: Interrelationships with each other and other writing skills'
Journal of Writing Research 6 (3) (2015) pp.199-231
ISSN: 2030-1006AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARAlthough handwriting is typically taught during early childhood and keyboarding may not be taught explicitly, both may be relevant to writing development in the later grades. Thus, Study 1 investigated automatic production of the ordered alphabet from memory for manuscript (unjoined), cursive (joined), and keyboard letter modes (alphabet 15 sec) and their relationships with each other and spelling and composing in typically developing writers in grades 4 to 7 (N = 113). Study 2 compared students with dysgraphia (impaired handwriting, n=27), dyslexia (impaired word spelling, n=40), or oral and written language learning disability (OWL LD) (impaired syntax composing, n=11) or controls without specific writing disabilities (n=10) in grades 4 to 9 (N=88) on the same alphabet 15 modes, manner of copying (best or fast), spelling, and sentence composing. In Study 1, sequential multilevel model regressions of predictor alphabet 15 letter production/selection modes on spelling and composition outcomes, measured annually from grade 4 to grade 7 (ages 9 to 13 years), showed that only the cursive mode uniquely, positively, and consistently predicted both spelling and composing in each grade. For composing, in grade 4 manuscript mode was positively predictive and in grades 5-7 keyboard selection was. In Study 2 all letter production modes correlated with each other and one’s best and fast sentence copying, spelling, and timed sentence composing. The groups with specific writing disabilities differed from control group on alphabet 15 manuscript mode, copy fast, and timed sentence composing. The dysgraphia and dyslexia groups differed on copying sentences in one’s best handwriting, with the dysgraphia group scoring lower. The educational and theoretical significance of the findings are discussed for multiple modes and manners of letter production/selection of the alphabet that support spelling and composing beyond the early grades in students with and without specific writing disabilities.
-
Sakr M, Connelly V, Wild M, 'Narrative in young children’s digital art-making'
Journal of Early Childhood Literacy 16 (3) (2015) pp.289-310
ISSN: 1468-7984 eISSN: 1741-2919AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARDigital technologies have material and social properties that have the potential to create new opportunities for children’s expressive arts practices. The presence and development of oral narratives in young children’s visual art-making on paper has been noted in previous research, but little is known about the narratives children create when they engage in digital art-making. How do young children construct narratives during digital art-making? How do the features of these narratives relate to the social and material properties of the digital resources they are using? How can looking at these narratives inform and enrich our understanding of children’s art-making in general? Drawing on a social semiotic perspective, these questions are explored through an in-depth analysis of narrative in three examples of 4-5 year olds’ digital art-making. On the basis of the analysis, features of oral narrative in young children’s digital art-making are suggested and these are linked to potentially influential properties of the digital resources. Being aware of these features and properties offers a starting point for thinking about what digital resources can offer in the context of young children’s art-making. The findings also prompt us to be aware of the diverse potentials that exist in children’s art-making practices regardless of the resources being used.
-
Sumner E, Connelly V, Barnett A L, 'The Influence of Spelling Ability on Vocabulary Choices when Writing for Children with Dyslexia'
Journal of Learning Disabilities 49 (3) (2014) pp.293-304
ISSN: 0022-2194 eISSN: 1538-4780AbstractPublished hereSpelling is a prerequisite to expressing vocabulary in writing. Research has shown that children with dyslexia are hesitant spellers when composing. This study aimed to determine whether the hesitant spelling of children with dyslexia, evidenced by frequent pausing, affects vocabulary choices when writing. A total of 31 children with dyslexia, mean age 9 years, were compared to typically developing groups of children: the first matched by age, the second by spelling ability. Oral vocabulary was measured and children completed a written and verbal compositional task. Lexical diversity comparisons were made across written and verbal compositions to highlight the constraint of having to select and spell words. A digital writing tablet recorded the writing. Children with dyslexia and the spelling-ability group made a high proportion of spelling errors and within-word pauses, and had a lower lexical diversity within their written compositions compared to their verbal compositions. The age-matched peers demonstrated the opposite pattern. Spelling ability and pausing predicted 53% of the variance in written lexical diversity of children with dyslexia, demonstrating the link between spelling and vocabulary when writing. Oral language skills had no effect. Lexical diversity correlated with written and verbal text quality for all groups. Practical implications are discussed and related to writing models.
-
Dockrell JE, Connelly V, Walter K, Critten S,, 'Assessing children's writing products: the role of curriculum based measures'
British Educational Research Journal 41 (4) (2014) pp.575-595
ISSN: 0141-1926 eISSN: 1469-3518AbstractPublished hereThe assessment of children's writing raises technical and practical challenges. In this paper we examine the potential use of a curriculum based measure for writing (CBM-W) to assess the written texts of pupils in Key Stage 2 (M age 107 months, range 88 to 125). Two hundred and thirty six Year three, five and six pupils completed a standardized assessment of writing quality. Pupils also responded to two writing probes, one expository and one narrative, at baseline and five months later. In response to the probe pupils wrote for five minutes. Transcribed texts were scored for productivity (total words produced, correct word sequences, number of punctuation marks and sentences produced) and accuracy (proportion of words spelled correctly, correct word sequences and punctuation marks). The CBM –W measures were differentially sensitive to development and showed construct validity as evidenced by their association with the norm- referenced test measuring writing quality. Change over time was also evident and significant differences between narrative and expository texts were found. Pupils with special educational needs scored significantly more poorly on the CBM-W. Potential implications for research and practice are considered.
-
Critten S, Connelly V, Dockrell JE, Walter K, 'Inflectional and derivational morphological spelling abilities of children with specific language impairment'
Frontiers in Psychology 5 (948) (2014)
ISSN: 1664-1078 eISSN: 1664-1078AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARChildren with Specific Language Impairment (SLI) are known to have difficulties with spelling but the factors that underpin these difficulties, are a matter of debate. The present study investigated the impact of oral language and literacy on the bound morpheme spelling abilities of children with SLI. Thirty-three children with SLI (9–10 years) and two control groups, one matched for chronological age (CA) and one for language and spelling age (LA) (aged 6–8 years) were given dictated spelling tasks of 24 words containing inflectional morphemes and 18 words containing derivational morphemes. There were no significant differences between the SLI group and their LA matches in accuracy or error patterns for inflectional morphemes. By contrast when spelling derivational morphemes the SLI group was less accurate and made proportionately more omissions and phonologically implausible errors than both control groups. Spelling accuracy was associated with phonological awareness and reading; reading performance significantly predicted the ability to spell both inflectional and derivational morphemes. The particular difficulties experienced by the children with SLI for derivational morphemes are considered in relation to reading and oral language.
-
Sumner E, Connelly V, Barnett AL, 'The Influence of Spelling Ability on Handwriting Production: Children with and without Dyslexia'
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 40 (5) (2014) pp.1441-1447
ISSN: 0278-7393AbstractPublished hereCurrent models of writing do not sufficiently address the complex relationship between the 2 transcription skills: spelling and handwriting. For children with dyslexia and beginning writers, it is conceivable that spelling ability will influence rate of handwriting production. Our aim in this study was to examine execution speed and temporal characteristics of handwriting when completing sentence-copying tasks that are free from composing demands and to determine the predictive value of spelling, pausing, and motor skill on handwriting production. Thirty-one children with dyslexia (Mage = 9 years 4 months) were compared with age-matched and spelling-ability matched children (Mage = 6 years 6 months). A digital writing tablet and Eye and Pen software were used to analyze handwriting. Children with dyslexia were able to execute handwriting at the same speed as the age-matched peers. However, they wrote less overall and paused more frequently while writing, especially within words. Combined spelling ability and within-word pausing accounted for over 76% of the variance in handwriting production of children with dyslexia, demonstrating that productivity relies on spelling capabilities. Motor skill did not significantly predict any additional variance in handwriting production. Reading ability predicted performance of the age-matched group, and pausing predicted performance for the spelling-ability group. The findings from the digital writing tablet highlight the interactive relationship between the transcription skills and how, if spelling is not fully automatized, it can constrain the rate of handwriting production. Practical implications are also addressed, emphasizing the need for more consideration to be given to what common handwriting tasks are assessing as a whole.
-
Dockrell JE, Connelly V, 'The role of oral language in underpinning the text generation difficulties in children with specific language impairment'
Journal of Research in Reading 38 (1) (2013) pp.18-34
ISSN: 0141-0423 eISSN: 1467-9817AbstractPublished hereChildren with specific language impairments (SLI) have difficulties in producing written text. It was hypothesised that the constraints on writing in children with SLI were similar to typically developing younger children with the same level of vocabulary knowledge. Twenty-three children with SLI (aged 10:5) were matched with 23 children for chronological age (CA) and 23 children for vocabulary levels (VC). Children with SLI performed significantly below their CA peers but not their VC peers on all aspects of writing including spelling. Regression analyses indicated that written text measures of spelling errors and oral language measures of vocabulary were significant predictors of writing products for both the children with SLI and their VC peers. This highlights the importance of oral and written language for the quality of children's written text and indicates that the writing of children with SLI was commensurate with their vocabulary and spelling levels. The results point to the role of both phonological and non-phonological processes in written text production in struggling writers.
-
Barnes J, Connelly V, Boubert L, Maravic K, 'Behavioural coping patterns in Parkinson's patients with visual hallucinations'
Journal of Neuropsychology 7 (2) (2013) pp.326-334
ISSN: 1748-6645AbstractVisual Hallucinations are considered to affect about 20%–40% of patients with Parkinson's disease. They are generally seen as a side effect of this long-term illness and can severely affect the daily quality of life of patients. The aim of this study was to determine the coping patterns or strategies used by patients and establish whether the phenomenology and behaviours used by patients enabled control of the phenomenon. Demographic and clinical variables were recorded, including motor measures, cognitive status, and depressive symptoms. Patient with hallucinations were at a more advance stage of the disease and displayed more depressive symptoms than their non-hallucinating counterparts. Most patients used more than one constructive coping strategy, the most common were simple behavioural strategies based around motor action or cognitive approaches resulting in visual modification. In addition, humour was a common technique used by the patients to deal with the phenomenon. Emotional responses varied between patients, but it was found that the actual content of the hallucination was not directly associated with whether it caused trouble to the patient, but perceived stress was strongly correlated with the subjective disturbing nature of visual hallucinations (VHs). This study gives insight into the role of cognitive-behavioural approaches when dealing with VHs and opens up avenues for future studies in helping patient to deal with hallucinations.Published here -
Connelly V, Barnett A, 'Children with dyslexia are slow writers because they pause more often and not because they are slow at handwriting execution'
Reading and Writing 26 (6) (2012) pp.991-1008
ISSN: 0922-4777 eISSN: 1573-0905AbstractIt is commonly assumed that children with dyslexia are slower at handwriting than other children. However, evidence of slow handwriting in children with dyslexia is very mixed. Thirty-one children with dyslexia, aged 9 years, were compared to both age-matched children and younger spelling-ability matched children. Participants completed an alphabet-writing task and a composition task on the surface of a digital writing tablet. Children with dyslexia wrote the same amount of letters per minute in the alphabet task but wrote fewer words per minute when composing their texts than children of the same age. Crucially, no differences were found between children with dyslexia and their same age peers for speed of handwriting execution, measured by the tablet, when writing the alphabet or composing their texts. However, children with dyslexia were found to pause within their compositions as often as the spelling ability matched group. Thus handwriting execution is not impaired in children with dyslexia. The slow writing that is typical of children with dyslexia is due to pausing more often when composing and is related to spelling ability. This may reflect processing problems in response to high cognitive load through having to contend with spelling and composing concurrently.Published here -
Connelly V, Dockrell J, Walter K, Critten S, 'Predicting the quality of composition and written language bursts from oral language, spelling, and handwriting skills in children with and without specific language impairment'
Written Communication 29 (3) (2012) pp.278-302
ISSN: 0741-0883 eISSN: 1552-8472AbstractPublished hereWriters typically produce their writing in bursts. In this article, the authors examine written language bursts in a sample of 33 children aged 11 years with specific language impairment. Comparisons of the children with specific language impairment with an age-matched group of typically developing children (n = 33) and a group of younger, language skill-matched children (n = 33) revealed the role of writing bursts as a key factor in differentiating writing competence. All the children produced the same number of writing bursts in a timed writing task. Children with specific language impairment produced a shorter number of words in each burst than did the age-matched group but the same as the language skill-matched group. For all groups, spelling accuracy and handwriting speed were significant predictors of burst length and text quality. The frequency of pauses at misspellings was related to shorter bursts. These results offer support to Hayes's model of text generation; namely, burst length is constrained by language and transcription skills.
-
Dockrell J, Connelly V, 'The role of oral language in underpinning the text generation difficulties in children with specific language impairment'
Journal of Research in Reading Early View (-) (2012) pp.--
ISSN: 0141-0423AbstractPublished hereChildren with specific language impairments (SLI) have difficulties in producing written text. It was hypothesised that the constraints on writing in children with SLI were similar to typically developing younger children with the same level of vocabulary knowledge. Twenty-three children with SLI (aged 10:5) were matched with 23 children for chronological age (CA) and 23 children for vocabulary levels (VC). Children with SLI performed significantly below their CA peers but not their VC peers on all aspects of writing including spelling. Regression analyses indicated that written text measures of spelling errors and oral language measures of vocabulary were significant predictors of writing products for both the children with SLI and their VC peers. This highlights the importance of oral and written language for the quality of children's written text and indicates that the writing of children with SLI was commensurate with their vocabulary and spelling levels. The results point to the role of both phonological and non-phonological processes in written text production in struggling writers.
-
Barnes J, Connelly V, Wiggs L, Boubert L, Maravic K, 'Sleep patterns in Parkinson's disease patients with visual hallucinations'
International Journal of Neuroscience 120 (8) (2010) pp.564-569
ISSN: 0020-7454AbstractPublished hereVisual hallucinations (VHs) in Parkinson" s disease (PD) can be a frequent and disturbing complication of the disease with 33% of PD patients undergoing long-term treatment experiencing VHs during the course of their illness. One line of evidence that is emerging as a possible risk factor in the occurrence of VHs is the sleep-wake cycle and sleep behavior in patients with PD. This study compared sleep patterns in a group of visually hallucinating Parkinson" s patients with a group of nonhallucinating PD patients and an age-matched control group. Nocturnal sleep was assessed by actigraphy and diaries, while daytime sleepiness and function were assessed by a battery of self-rating sleep questionnaires. Compared with the control group both patient groups had more sleep-related problems and significantly altered sleep patterns, as measured by both actigraphy and sleep questionnaires. Patients who hallucinated however slept less than nonhallucinating patients and also had increased awakenings after sleep onset, reduced sleep efficiency, and increased daytime sleepiness. We propose that VHs in some PD patients may be a symptom of poor sleep and prolonged daytime sleepiness, suggesting that arousal may play a role in the genesis of the hallucination phenomenon.
-
Dockrell JE, Lindsay G, Connelly V, 'The impact of specific language impairment on adolescents' written text'
Exceptional Children 75 (4) (2009) pp.427-446
ISSN: 0014-4029AbstractThis study examined the writing performance of 58 students with a history of specific language impairment, assessing them at ages 8, 11, 12, 14, and 16 to evaluate longitudinal trajectories of writing performance and relationships with oral language, reading, and handwriting fluency. At age 16, participants continued to experience problems with oral language and literacy: Their writing evidenced short texts, poor sentence structure, and difficulties with ideas and organization. Concurrent measures of vocabulary and spelling were significant factors in explaining writing performance. Handwriting fluency remained a particular difficulty for the current cohort and directly affected writing performance. Path analysis indicated that previous levels of literacy mediated the impact of oral language skills.Published here -
Thompson G B, Connelly V, Fletcher-Flinn C, Hodson S J, 'The nature of skilled adult reading varies with type of instruction in childhood'
Memory & Cognition 37 (2) (2009) pp.223-234
ISSN: 0090-502XAbstractPublished hereDoes the type of reading instruction experienced during the initial years at school have any continuing effect on the ways in which adults read words? The question has arisen in current discussions about computational models of mature word-reading processes. We tested predicted continuing effects by comparing matched samples of skilled adult readers of English who had received explicit phonics instruction in childhood and those who had not. In responding to nonwords that can receive alternative legitimate pronunciations, those adults having childhood phonics instruction used more regular grapheme-phoneme correspondences that were context free and used fewer vocabulary-based contextually dependent correspondences than did adults who had no phonics instruction. These differences in regularization of naming responses also extended to some low-frequency words. This apparent cognitive footprint of childhood phonics instruction is a phenomenon requiring consideration when researchers attempt to model adult word reading and when they select participants to test the models.
-
Thompson GB, McKay M, Fletcher-Flinn C, Connelly V, Kaa RT, Ewing J, 'Do children who acquire word reading without explicit phonics employ compensatory learning? Issues of phonological recoding, lexical orthography, and fluency'
Reading and Writing 21 (5) (2008) pp.505-537
ISSN: 0922-4777 eISSN: 1573-0905AbstractTwo studies were conducted across three countries to examine samples of beginning readers without systematic explicit phonics who had reached the same level of word reading accuracy as comparison samples with high and moderate explicit phonics. Had they employed any compensatory learning to reach that level? Four hypotheses of compensatory learning or performance were tested on the samples, all of which represented the lower half of the normative distribution of word reading accuracy. The two samples without explicit phonics received teaching that centered on story text reading and some receptive phonics that arose from this text reading. They did not compensate by relatively greater use of a larger psycholinguistic grain size in the form of rime units. Nor did they compensate by trading off comprehension for text word reading accuracy. In a microtraining study, they showed no compensation in proficiency of initial learning of lexical orthographic representations. For all samples, this initial learning was less effective with spelling than reading training trials. In reading text, the samples without explicit phonics did not compensate by trading off speed for accuracy, or comprehension. On the contrary, they read text faster than the explicit phonics samples. The extra classroom instruction time available to them for text reading, with the consequential extra exposure and practice of word reading, would explain this result.Published here -
Dockrell JE, Lindsay G, Connelly V, Mackie C, 'Constraints in the Production of Written Text in Children With Specific Language Impairments'
Exceptional Children 73 (2) (2007) pp.147-164
ISSN: 0014-4029 eISSN: 2163-5560AbstractThe writing performance of 64 elementary school children with a history of specific language impairment was examined to evaluate both the nature of the children's difficulties with writing and the relationship between oral language, reading, and writing. Children were assessed at age 8 on a range of language, literacy, and cognitive measures and reassessed at age 10 when they completed a standardized writing measure. At age 10 the children continued to experience problems with oral language and language levels were significantly poorer than nonverbal skills. Writing was characterized by short texts with poor sentence structure and little evidence of ideas and organization. Both concurrent measures of receptive vocabulary and reading skill were significant factors in explaining levels of written language.Published here -
Connelly V, Dockrell J E, Barnett J, 'A pilot intervention to improve the structural quality of exam essay writing in UK undergraduate psychology students'
Psychology Teaching Review 12 (1) (2006) pp.43-56
ISSN: 0965-948X eISSN: 2396-9571AbstractPsychology undergraduates need to produce good quality essays in order to succeed at university. Students find the transition to university writing difficult. Using a rubric, a profile of student weakness in psychology essay writing was described. The students were generally poor at the structural organisation of their essays. A pilot intervention to improve essay structure was designed based on the profile of student weakness. The intervention was trialled with a group of 23 psychology undergraduate students. Those students who took part in the intervention programme had higher exam essay marks with longer and more clearly structured essays after the programme than a matched group of controls. The intervention group students produced essays that linked evidence more clearly to topics and provided better conclusion sections. Psychology tutors were able to show improvement in student writing with a short explicit lesson based on a discipline-specific profile of writing. However, there was no improvement in vocabulary for linking topics and the structure of the introductions remained poor. Ideas for the further development of this intervention are discussed.Published here -
Connelly V, MacLean M, Barnes J, 'Contribution of lower-order letter and word fluency skills to written composition of college students with or without dyslexia'
Developmental Neuropsychology 29 (1) (2006) pp.175-196
ISSN: 8756-5641AbstractThere has been a growth in students with dyslexia attending university. These students commonly rate writing as one of their greatest problem areas. Our research set out to describe the effects of dyslexia on the writing skills of students compared to age-matched peers and a spelling-skill-matched group. Generally, the texts of the students with dyslexia were poorer than age controls but not poorer than the spelling-skill controls. However, there were no major differences in "higher order" skills such as ideas and organization with the chronological age controls, only in "lower order" transcription skills such as spelling and handwriting fluency. The students with dyslexia made more spelling errors in their essays than one would predict given their dictated spelling skills.Published here -
Connelly V, Campbell S, MacLean M, Barnes J, 'Contribution of Lower Order Skills to the Written Composition of College Students With and Without Dyslexia'
Developmental Neuropsychology 29 (1) (2006) pp.175-196
ISSN: 8756-5641 eISSN: 1532-6942AbstractThere has been a growth in students with dyslexia attending university. These students commonly rate writing as one of their greatest problem areas. Our research set out to describe the effects of dyslexia on the writing skills of students compared to age-matched peers and a spelling-skill-matched group. Generally, the texts of the students with dyslexia were poorer than age controls but not poorer than the spelling-skill controls. However, there were no major differences in "higher order" skills such as ideas and organization with the chronological age controls, only in "lower order" transcription skills such as spelling and handwriting fluency. The students with dyslexia made more spelling errors in their essays than one would predict given their dictated spelling skills.Published here -
Connelly V, Dockrell JE, Barnett J, 'The slow handwriting of undergraduate students constrains overall performance in exam essays'
Educational Psychology 25 (1) (2005) pp.99-107
ISSN: 0144-3410 eISSN: 1469-5820AbstractUndergraduates producing handwritten essays in university exams need to transcribe information onto the page in a rapid and efficient manner under considerable time pressure. In fact, capacity models of the writing process predict that the more automated students can make the transcription process then the more resources will be available for higher order writing processes. This study examined the impact of low level handwriting processes on undergraduate writing in pressurised and unpressurised tasks. Students completed a measure of handwriting fluency and provided samples of writing from exam conditions and a formative class essay. The results indicated that, compared to a class essay, exam writing was constrained by the low level writing skill of handwriting fluency. Surprisingly, it was found that the undergraduates were very slow writers whose writing speed was equivalent to published fluency data on 11-year-old schoolchildren. The relationships between handwriting fluency and writing quality were also very similar to those of published data on 11-year-old children, with handwriting fluency accounting for large amounts of the variance in writing quality and tutor marks for exam answers. The results of the current study indicate that lower level processes constrain the higher level performance of undergraduate students to a significant extent. This limitation needs to be considered when undergraduate exams are designed and inferences drawn from exam performance.Published here -
Connelly V, Fletcher-Flinn CM, Thompson GB, Hodson S, 'Why Instructional Methods Can No Longer Be Ignored in Theories of the Reading Process'
Australian Journal of Psychology 56 (supplement) (2005) pp.115-115
ISSN: 0004-9530 eISSN: 1742-9536Published here -
Connelly V, Gee D, Walsh E, 'A comparison of keyboarded and handwritten compositions and the relationship with transcription speed'
British Journal of Educational Psychology 77 (2) (2002) pp.479-492
ISSN: 0007-0998 eISSN: 2044-8279AbstractBackground. It is well established that handwriting fluency constrains writing quality by limiting resources for higher order processes such as planning and reviewing. According to the"simple view of writing" then slow keyboarding speed should hinder the quality of keyboarded essay compositions in the same way that slow handwriting hinders handwritten essay compositions. Given a lack of touch-typing instruction in UK schools it was hypothesized that children's written compositions produced via the keyboard would be worse than produced by hand. Aims. To extend the work of Christensen (2004) and Rogers and Case-Smith (2002) by examining the relationship between handwriting fluency and keyboarding fluency throughout the primary school and studying the link between word-processed compositional quality and keyboarding fluency. Samples and methods. The handwriting fluency and keyboarding fluency of 300 children in primary school were measured. Year S and year 6 children completed a measure of compositional quality by hand and by keyboard. Results and comment. There was a high correlation between handwriting and keyboarding speed and handwriting speed was consistently faster than keyboarding speed across all ages. Only a small minority of children in years 5 and 6 had faster keyboarding than handwriting speed. Results showed that children's compositional quality was superior in the handwritten scripts as opposed to the keyboarded scripts. Keyboarded scripts were up to 2 years behind handwritten scripts in development. Writing by keyboard does not necessarily lead to improvements in script quality, compared with handwritten scripts. Explicit keyboarding instruction (touch-typing) is needed to develop keyboarding fluency and unlock the full potential of the word processor for children's writing.Published here -
Connelly V, 'Graphophonemic awareness, in adults after instruction in phonic generalisations'
Learning and Instruction 12 (6) (2002) pp.627-649
ISSN: 0959-4752AbstractA number of studies have shown, counter-intuitively, that skilled adult readers find it difficult to correctly relate phonemes to graphemes in written words. In most adults this is of little consequence. However, teachers are required to show children how to relate phonemes to graphemes when teaching reading. This study investigated whether a brief period of instruction would improve the ability of adults in this task. Results showed a general level of improvement relative to controls. Exposure alone to the task had no effect. Improvement for the instructed group was not very large and there was difficulty relating phonemes to graphemes which had more than one letter. It may be that trainee teachers need instruction to improve knowledge of grapheme phoneme correspondence so that they can pass such knowledge onto young readers.Published here -
Connelly V, Johnston R, Thompson GB, 'The effect of phonics instruction on the reading comprehension of beginning readers'
Reading and Writing 14 (5) (2001) pp.423-457
ISSN: 0922-4777 eISSN: 1573-0905AbstractThis study investigated whether two groups of 6-year-old beginning readers taught to read by a phonics and by a "book experience'' non-phonics approach would differ in reading comprehension as well as the processes of word recognition. The two groups were matched for word recognition but despite this, the phonics taught children had higher reading comprehension. Phonics taught children produced more contextually appropriate errors, and in both single word and text reading made more spoken attempts at reading unknown words. The non-phonics taught children had much faster reading reaction times to familiar words but they scored less in phoneme segmentation and nonword reading tasks. Compared with the non-phonics group, the phonics group spent more time in attempts at identifying unknown words and this included using contextual information, which apparently resulted in more rehearsal of the meaning of the story text and hence better reading comprehension performance.Published here -
Connelly V, Johnston RS, Thompson GB, 'The influence of instructional technique on the reading acquisition routes of beginning readers'
International Journal of Psychology 31 (3/4) (1996) pp.2481-2481
ISSN: 0020-7594 eISSN: 1464-066XAbstractPublished hereScottish beginning readers taught by a phonic method are compared with New Zealand children taught by a book experience approach. Samples are matched for reading age, chronological age, time at school, vocabulary knowledge and digit span. Tests of word recognition, phonological skills, reaction time and comprehension were administered. Results show differences in nonword and irregular word reading, phonological skill, comprehension, reaction time and error production. Despite being matched on general ability both groups show differences in subskills associated with reading that would appear to stem from how they have been taught. This work has implications for models of reading development.
-
Connelly V, Johnston RS, Thompson GB, 'The influence of instructional-technique on the reading acquisition routes of beginning readers'
International Journal of Psychology 31 (3-4) (1996) pp.2481-2481
ISSN: 0020-7594 eISSN: 1464-066X
Books
-
Ben-Ari E, Connelly V, (ed.), Contemporary Military Reserves: Between the Civilian and Military Worlds, Routledge (2023)
ISBN: 9781032307206 eISBN: 9781003306399AbstractPublished hereThis book offers a comparative study of military reserves in contemporary democracies.
A combination of budgetary pressures, new missions and emerging military roles during the past three decades has led the armed forces of democracies to rethink the training and use of reserve forces. Moreover, reservists have become central to the armed forces as part of moves toward “total” or “comprehensive” defense. Despite this, a scholarly bias toward studying regulars and conscripts means that reservists and reserve soldiers continue to receive only marginal attention. This volume fills that lacuna through a series of country studies examining how best to understand the peculiarities of reservist service. In contrast to regulars and conscripts, reservists are marked by their dual management of civilian and military careers, different family dynamics, diverse motivations and commitment to the armed forces, the material and non-material incentives they are offered, and their place in the political sphere. This volume suggests two frames to make sense of such differences: first, it looks at reservists as “transmigrants” traveling between the military and civilian worlds; and, second, it analyzes the multiple informal “contracts” and negotiations that bind them to the military. All the chapters adopt these conceptualizations, granting the volume a common focus and integrative frame.
The volume will be of much interest to students of military and strategic studies, civil–military relations, sociology and International Relations. -- Provided by publisher.
-
Miller B, McCardle P, Connelly V, (ed.), Writing development in struggling learners: Understanding the needs of writers across the lifecourse, Brill (2018)
ISBN: 9789004345812 eISBN: 9789004346369AbstractIn Writing Development in Struggling Learners, international writing researchers provide critical insights into the development of writing skills for individuals who struggle to become proficient writers. This edited volume takes a life course view and examines concepts for development of writing skills with a focus on where learners struggle, why this may occur for those without and without specific learning disorders (SLDs), how to identify these learners and what we can do to facilitate efficient writing. Throughout the volume, struggling learners are presented with a holistic lens; contributors succinctly synthesize the literature base and present insights into the current state of the science and areas of future need and advancement. -
Wood C, Connelly V, (ed.), Contemporary perspectives on reading and spelling, Routledge (2009)
ISBN: 9780415497176AbstractWith contributions from leading international researchers, Contemporary Perspectives on Reading and Spelling offers a critique of current thinking on the research literature into reading, reading comprehension and writing. Each paper in this volume provides an account of empirical research that challenges aspects of accepted models and widely accepted theories about reading and spelling. This book develops the argument for a need to incorporate less widely cited research into popular accounts of written language development and disability, challenging the idea that the development of a universal theory of written language development is attainable. The arguments within the book are explored in three parts: -overarching debates in reading and spelling -reading and spelling across languages -written language difficulties and approaches to teaching. Opening up the existing debates, and incorporating psychological theory and the politics surrounding the teaching and learning of reading and spelling, this edited collection offers some challenging points for reflection about how the discipline of psychology as a whole approaches the study of written language skills. Highlighting ground-breaking new perspectives, this book forms essential reading for all researchers and practitioners with a focus on the development of reading and spelling skills.Published here -
Connelly V, Barnett AL, (ed.), Teaching and learning writing, British Psychological Society (2009)
ISBN: 9781854334909AbstractThis monograph presents recent international research on the teaching and learning of writing. The purpose of the monograph is to inform education professionals about this exciting new research which we hope will, in turn, contribute to improved practice and performance in the classroom.The studies in this monograph fall into two broad and overlapping areas of interest. The first is concerned with charting performance and development in writing skills in typical and in special populations. Topics covered include writing as discovery; linguistic development in writing; the writing of children with language impairments; unexpectedly good and poor spellers; and brain activation and idea generation. The second area is concerned directly with assessment, assistance and instruction in writing. Topics covered here range from drawing lessons from research for evidence based writing practice in the classroom; self-regulated strategy development in writing; the development of a new handwriting speed test; and the use of information technology in the classroom for struggling writers.
-
Connelly V, Barnett A L, Dockrell J, Tolmie A, Teaching and learning writing, British Psychological Society (2009)
ISBN: 9781854334909AbstractThis monograph presents recent international research on the teaching and learning of writing. The purpose of the monograph is to inform education professionals about this exciting new research which we hope will, in turn, contribute to improved practice and performance in the classroom. The studies in this monograph fall into two broad and overlapping areas of interest. The first is concerned with charting performance and development in writing skills in typical and in special populations. Topics covered include writing as discovery; linguistic development in writing; the writing of children with language impairments; unexpectedly good and poor spellers; and brain activation and idea generation. The second area is concerned directly with assessment, assistance and instruction in writing. Topics covered here range from drawing lessons from research for evidence based writing practice in the classroom; self-regulated strategy development in writing; the development of a new handwriting speed test; and the use of information technology in the classroom for struggling writers. Taken together, these papers emphasize the complexity of the writing process and the various levels of analysis that can be used to inform practice; ranging from the purely biological through to meta-analytic techniques that illustrate which particular teaching interventions potentially have the most impact on writing development and why.
Book chapters
-
Griffith J, Connelly V, Catignani S, Johansson E, 'Reservists and Veterans: Viewed from Within and Without' in Handbook of Military Sciences, Springer Link (2020)
eISBN: 9783030028664AbstractPublished here Open Access on RADARThis chapter describes two important groups relative to military service – reservists and veterans. Definitions are provided regarding who is a member of each group. A summary of past and current research findings for each group is provided. The summary is organized by investigative topics or themes, which provide the current scope of the field for reservists and for veterans. Finally, approaches to the study of reservists and veterans are described, along with challenges – both substantively and methodologically – for future research studies. These serve as fertile areas for improvements and investigations in future research studies.
-
Sakr M, Connelly V, Wild M, 'Teachers’ use of writing in displays of young children’s artwork' in Archer A, Breur E (ed.), Multimodality in Writing: The state of the art in theory, methodology and pedagogy, Brill (2015)
ISBN: 9789004296572 eISBN: 9789004297197AbstractChapter 12Published here -
Connelly V, 'Integrating writing and oral language disorders: Perspectives of a Writing Researcher' in Writing development in children with hearing loss,dyslexia or oral language problems: implications for assessment and instruction, Oxford University Press (2014)
ISBN: 9780199827282AbstractPublished hereThis chapter is a reflection on the research presented throughout the book and poses questions about where writing and oral language research needs to go next. The author makes the cases that true integration of writing and oral-language research requires agreed definitions, common measurement and terminology, and that these are needed if new and emerging technical tools to investigate writing processes, such as digital writing capture, are to make a difference. There is also a call for more cross-linguistic research and for researchers to continue to challenge writing theory by examining writing and oral language difficulties in those children with difficulties such as dyslexia. There also needs to be a clearer integration of research with the “task environment” and, in particular, classroom instruction.
-
Dockrell JE, Connelly V, Lindsay G, Mackie C, 'The Impact of Oral Language Skills on Children’s Production of Written Text' in Learning to Write Effectively: Current Trends in European Research, Brill (2012)
ISBN: 978-1-78052-928-8 431 eISBN: 978-1-78052-929-5AbstractChapter 1.00.10Published here
Other publications
-
Connelly V, Sumner E, O'Rourke L, 'How being a poor speller can seriously lift your talents as a writer', (2019)
Published here -
Connelly V, 'The Myths Surrounding Veterans', (2018)
Published here Open Access on RADAR -
Connelly V, Morrison Z, Fear N, Hennelly S, Smith J, 'Support to the Families of Reservists: An Investigation of Needs, Current Provision and Gaps. Final Report DHCSTC.', (2017)
-
Connelly V., 'Written submission to House of Commons Defence Committee inquiry on SDSR 2015 and the Army August 2016', (2016)
-
Parry E, Connelly V, Robinson D, Robinson Z, Taylor C, 'Integration of the Whole Force: Understanding barriers and enablers to maximise task and team performance.Final Report DHCSTC.', (2016)
-
Parry E, Morrison Z, Robinson D, Snowdon E, Swift S, Rennick K, Booth C, Connelly V, 'Awareness, Attraction and Retention of the Reserve Forces. Final Report, DHCSTC', (2016)
Professional information
Memberships of professional bodies
- Fellow of the Inter University Seminar (IUS) on Armed Forces and Society
- Member of European Research Group on Military and Society (ERGOMAS)
- Advisory board member King’s Centre for Military Health Research (KCMHR). 2015-2017, 2020-2022.
- External Examiner for MSc in War and Psychiatry, Kings College London (2016-2021)
- Member of Expert Advisory Group for UK Veterans Family Study 2021-2022
- Member of Academic Advisory Board to Forces in Mind Trust (FiMT) Research Centre 2021-ongoing
- Invited Chair of the British Dyslexia Association International Research Conference Panel 2018-2021
- European Co-ordinator of the Special Interest Group on Writing (http://www.sig-writing.org) as part of the European Association for Research in Learning and Instruction 2013-2017.
- Trustee of Berkshire Yeomanry Museum. I am a trustee (treasurer) of this small, registered museum that was founded in 1975. The museum is in Windsor and is open to the public free of charge.